Volume 19: pp. 113-120

Envisioning the Future of Comparative Cognition Through an Undergraduate Lens

Deirdre B. Yeater

Department of Psychology, Sacred Heart University

Heather Manitzas Hill

Department of Psychology, St. Mary’s University

Lauren Highfill

Department of Psychology and Animal Studies, Eckerd College

Reading Options


Abstract

This commentary provides an updated summary of how comparative cognition is approached by researchers based at primarily undergraduate institutions. Because of the difficulties in supporting traditional laboratories, we highlight a number of nontraditional research methodologies that can be accessed by undergraduate students. We also provide advantages and disadvantages of these alternate methodologies. We promote the need for interdisciplinary study of animal cognition topics in in situ and ex situ settings, as well as call for an expansion of cognitive abilities for application to animal welfare and conservation efforts.

Keywordsundergraduates, comparative cognition, zoos, PUI, citizen science, nontraditional animal laboratories

Author Note Deirdre Yeater, Department of Psychology, Sacred Heart University, 5151 Park Avenue, Fairfield, CT 06825.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Deirdre Yeater at yeaterd@sacredheart.edu


Although the comparative cognition field faces challenges in funding and resources, emerging technologies and interdisciplinary collaborations offer hope. We explore topics critical to the advancement of comparative cognition while highlighting practicalities needed to achieve this goal. From our primarily undergraduate institution (PUI) perspective, we examine the current state of the discipline and share challenges and successes we have encountered through more than 20 years of research.

Previous Reflections on Comparative Cognition

The status of comparative cognition as a field has been addressed by many authors (e.g., Bauer et al., 2020; Beran, 2023; Beran et al., 2014; Farrar et al., 2020, 2021; Krasheninnikova et al., 2020). These articles shared several themes, including advocating for broader species inclusion, larger samples, replication studies, and more sensitive and comparative-friendly research methodologies. Additionally, an emphasis on social cognition and the consideration of individual differences were recurring suggestions.

Large-scale collaborative efforts have emerged under the Many Projects umbrella, which facilitates larger samples and encourages replication (ManyBirds: Lambert et al., 2022; ManyDogs Project et al., 2023; ManyPrimates et al., 2019). As a proof of concept of these collaborations, ManyPrimates successfully conducted a partial replication study on short-term memory with multiple species of primates (N = 41) and the largest number of individual primates ever tested (N = 421; ManyPrimates et al., 2022). Beyond the collaborative projects, the range of species under study has expanded to include less studied groups such as reptiles, fish, insects, and invertebrates (Burghardt, 2010, 2015; Matsubara et al., 2017). This diversity of species offers opportunities for testing hypotheses not feasible with birds or mammals (Matsubara et al., 2017). Additionally, cognitive research is increasingly conducted in nontraditional settings, such as zoos, aquariums, farms, and in the wild (Hopper, 2017; Maple, 2016; Pritchard et al., 2016; Vonk et al., 2015).

Personal Reflections on the Field of Comparative Cognition

As professors at PUIs who conduct research in zoological institutions with marine mammals (e.g., dolphins, belugas, killer whales, sea lions) and charismatic zoo animals (e.g., elephants, orangutans, otters), we have provided myriad opportunities for undergraduate students to study animal behavior and cognition by exposing them to the research process, from developing ideas and apparatus, to data collection and analysis, and culminating in dissemination. High-impact practices, such as these, have been shown to develop interest and transform students into future passionate researchers (i.e., Hill & Karlin, 2019; Karlin et al., 2022; Lanning & Brown, 2019). 

Although comparative cognition appears to thrive in many areas, issues persist, especially within PUIs. Abramson’s (2015) call for recruiting undergraduates as the next generation of comparative psychologists resonates with our observations in the field of comparative cognition. Notably, this discipline is often overlooked in psychology programs, lacks accessible textbooks for undergraduates, and faces a shortage of graduate programs. Whether because of funding issues, interest, or the need to justify animal-model research, many comparative programs have shifted toward neuroscience and working on translational research questions. We believe that there is room for both comparative psychology, specifically comparative cognition, and neuroscience, and in fact these two subfields should be complementary.

Alternatives to Traditional Laboratories

Traditionally, undergraduate students have been exposed to animal cognition through animal laboratories (“labs”). However, budgetary constraints and lack of support staff are prompting institutions (including our own) to move away from housing animal colonies on campus. We emphasize the importance of traditional animal labs for fundamental research, but we acknowledge the need for alternatives when on-campus housing is impractical.

One growing trend involves setting up “nontraditional” animal labs where animal subjects come for a day of research. For example, many canine cognition labs have developed over the past several years (Byosiere & Prasad-Shreckengast, 2022; Furlong et al., 2015; Wynne, 2016), where the animals are brought to a study location for a brief task. Benefits to this approach include less overhead in terms of animal care, responsibility, and training involved, but this approach limits the types of cognitive questions that can be tested. Although bringing animals to a study site for the short term may affect control over factors such as history or genetics, any consistent cognitive findings would be more universal because of variation. Other nontraditional locations for research have included in homes, animal shelters (Thielke & Udell, 2019), and doggie day care centers (Salzer & Reed, 2022; Trisko & Smuts, 2015).

To advance the field, exploring nontraditional locations for data collection is crucial. Encouraging partnerships with zoos, nature centers, aquariums, rescue centers, animal shelters, and doggie day care centers broadens research possibilities. The use of ex situ sites has been championed by Maple (2016), who paved the way for research partnerships between zoological and academic institutions. Research in zoos not only leads to a deeper understanding of the behavior and cognition of animals but also provides meaningful enrichment, which improves overall welfare. Research conducted at zoos, nature centers, or similar facilities can engage the public with science (e.g., Harley et al., 2010). This engagement propels the conservation missions of institutions and results in conservation behavioral changes in patrons (e.g., Godinez & Fernandez, 2019; Grajal et al., 2017). Despite these benefits, collecting data in zoos and aquariums creates several challenges, including limited access to animals (Highfill & Yeater, 2018) or limited trial opportunities. Often, the number of student research assistants is limited by facility protocols. Despite these challenges, once a project is in place, the benefits often outweigh the initial difficulties, as demonstrated by the success of a longitudinal study of beluga behavior across multiple facilities over more than 15 years (Hill et al., 2021).

Another nontraditional lab space is the use of citizen science, in which laypersons participate more intentionally in research, such as dog owners testing their dogs at home (Beran, 2023). Citizen science increases the scope of individuals to be accessed for engagement in cognitive research, as evidenced by a dog cognition study that recruited more than 500 citizen scientists to replicate a traditional lab-based study (Stewart et al., 2015). Since then, growing evidence has shown that citizen science is a useful technique to gather animal cognition data sets. As an example from one of our labs, Willgohs and colleagues (2022) had amateur dog trainers participate in research by filming training and experimental sessions on creativity. Researchers at University of California, San Diego are currently reviewing home videos of the famous Bunny the Shepadoodle, whose owner claims Bunny can press buttons to communicate with humans (Inside Edition, 2020). Although these examples demonstrate the possibility of this nontraditional lab space, there are unwanted consequences, including lack of control, variation in implementation of procedure, and potential for fabrication of data by untrained data collectors. However, if the project is simple enough, data from individuals may be aggregated.

Expanding Methodologies to Many Species

Replications, a great option for undergraduate student participation, can be extended to different settings and species, which provides opportunities for comparative methodologies to be implemented. Another extension from traditional lab settings is to perform replications of cognitive research to similar species in natural habitats. Visual laterality in cetaceans was assessed both in managed care (Hill et al., 2016; Lilley et al., 2020; Manitzas Hill et al., 2022; Yeater et al., 2014, 2017) and in the wild (belugas: Karenina, Giljov, Baranov, et al., 2010; Karenina, Giljov, Malashichev, et al., 2010; killer whales: Karenina et al., 2013; striped dolphins: Siniscalchi et al., 2012). Highfill and Kuczaj (2010) examined the construct of personality in wild and captive dolphins. These examples and more were explored in a 2016 special issue by the International Journal of Comparative Psychology (Hill et al., 2016), and more recently at a workshop on how comparative cognition may be applied to conservation questions during the Society for Marine Mammalogy biennial meeting (Harley et al., 2022). As Beran (2023) highlighted, comparative cognition needs to broaden the species tested. One promising avenue is the exploration of cognitive questions in livestock (i.e., Horback & Parsons, 2022; Nawroth et al., 2019). This approach not only addresses phylogenetic and functional questions but also enhances our understanding of the welfare of these animals.

We also encourage extending and validating research methodologies to other species and across settings. The use of the same methodologies with different species is a truly comparative approach and more informative, ultimately. For example, using the same methodology in various settings, we have studied creativity in dogs (Willgohs et al., 2022), dolphins, killer whales (Dudzinski et al., 2018; Hill et al., 2022), and preschoolers (Melzer et al., 2022); violation of expectations in belugas, bottlenose dolphins, Pacific white-sided dolphins (Manitzas Hill et al., 2022), rough-toothed dolphins (Lilley et al., 2018), and dogs (Yeater et al., 2024); long-term memory and problem solving (otters species and dogs; DeLong et al., 2024). The advantages of conducting comparative research enable the investigation of similar problems with flexible but standardized methodologies that can be adapted to facility needs. However, standardizing the methodology across different settings and accessing different groups of animals can be challenging.

Future Directions

Both lab-based studies and observational research remain important tools in comparative cognition. Lab-based research enables specific questions about comparative cognition topics to be examined with control and manipulation while observational research validates these topics in ecologically valid settings and provides undergraduates opportunities to develop research skills. Observational research can be performed in various settings, offering less invasive approaches and easier approval processes. Topics that lend themselves to observational and lab-based research are play, innovative behavior, personality, and problem solving.

One exciting application of comparative cognition research is to address animal welfare needs, which may then enhance conservation measures. The Five Domain model (Mellor et al., 2020) emphasizes several areas of welfare that should be addressed for animals in managed care, including cognitive engagement. Comparative cognition research could simulate natural problems animals face in situ or ex situ, such as farms or zoos, which helps to evaluate the well-being of these animals (Clark, 2017; Jaakkola, 2023; Mehrkam et al., 2020; Nawroth et al., 2019). When facilities engage their animals in cognitive research projects, the animals, guests, and trainers benefit (Harley et al., 2010). 

Comparing studies in managed care and with wild populations is necessary for the future of comparative cognition (Beran et al., 2014; Kuczaj, 2010; Miller et al., 2013). This task is becoming more realistic with advances in technology. Drones deliver both photographic and real-time videographic evidence for comparative cognition, including foraging efforts, social behavior, decision making, and navigation, while being used as experimental conditions to assess the effect of anthropogenic actions on animals in nature (Sueur & Pelé, 2023). Additionally, different aspects of artificial intelligence are providing more efficient processing of images, video recordings, sound recordings, and sensory data that enable behaviors to be modeled more effectively, which may produce insights into cognitive processes. Applying cognitive research questions and techniques to solving human–animal interaction issues could inform wildlife management practices. For example, in Sarasota Bay, Florida, cognitive research could be used to countercondition dolphins that get too close to people and beg for food (Harley et al., 2022). Techniques using behavioral principles of aversion or reinforcement are implemented in Africa to help elephants avoid high-poaching areas (The Ranch, 2021). Ultimately, though, modeling is dependent upon the information gathered from the field and must be validated before using it to predict behavior and apply to animal welfare. 

The field has many unanswered research questions, which makes for exciting future explorations. We believe that to be truly comparative, we should strive to directly compare multiple species with the same methodology (e.g., Krasheninnikova et al., 2020). Synthesizing information gathered from in situ or ex situ settings while exploring parallels with artificial intelligence systems is our future. Questions remain about the evolutionary history and environmental pressures for most cognitive abilities (Shettleworth, 2009). Research should emphasize the ecological importance of cognition in topics such as spatial cognition, long-term memory, cognitive load, decision making, and emotion. The comparative cognition field should continue to promote cross-disciplinary, and not anthropomorphic or over-generalized, conclusions.

References

Abramson, C. (2015). A crisis in comparative psychology: Where have all the undergraduates gone? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 1500. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01500

Bauer, G. B., Cook, P. F., & Harley, H. E. (2020). The relevance of ecological transitions to intelligence in marine mammals. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article 2053. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02053

Beran, M. J. (2023). Congratulations to Animal Cognition on its 50th birthday! Some thoughts on the last 50 years of animal cognition research. Animal Cognition, 26, 13–23. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-022-01706-5

Beran, M. J., Parrish, A.E., Perdue, B.M., & Washburn, D. A. (2014). Comparative cognition: Past, present, and future. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 27(1), 3–30. https://doi.org/10.46867/ijcp.2014.27.01.07

Burghardt, G. M. (2010). The comparative reach of play and brain: Perspective, evidence, and implications. American Journal of Play, 2(3), 338–356.

Burghardt, G. M. (2015). Play in fishes, frogs and reptiles. Current Biology, 25(1), R9–R10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.10.027

Byosiere, S. E., & Prasad-Shreckengast, S. (2022). Canine cognition. In J. Vonk & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of animal cognition and behavior (pp. 932–941). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-55065-7_1700

Clark, F. E. (2017). Cognitive enrichment and welfare: Current approaches and future directions. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 4(1), 52–71. https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.05.02.2017

DeLong, C., Yeater, D., Manitazs Hill, H., & Melzer, D. (2024). ManyOtters1: A Pilot Study on Long-Term Memory Using Puzzle Feeders [Unpublished raw data].

Dudzinski, K., Yeater, D., Bolton, T., Eskelinen, H., & Hill. H. (2018). Defining creativity and confirming understanding of the concept in dolphins: Research and training perspectives. Aquatic Mammals, 44(4), 426–436. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.44.4.2018.426

Farrar, B. G., Boeckle, M., & Clayton, N. S. (2020). Replications in comparative cognition: What should we expect and how can we improve? Animal Behavior and Cognition, 7(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.26451/abc.07.01.02.2020

Farrar, B. G., Voudouris, K., & Clayton, N. S. (2021). Replications, comparisons, sampling and the problem of representativeness in animal cognition research. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 8(2), Article 273. https://doi.org/10.26451/abc.08.02.14.2021

Furlong, E., AuBuchon, S., Kraut, J., Joiner, N., Knowles, J., Lewis, K., Win, M., & Furlong, J. (2015). From crisis to crowd control. Commentary: A crisis in comparative psychology: Where have all the undergraduates gone? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 1729. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01729

Godinez, A. M., & Fernandez, E. J. (2019). What is the zoo experience? How zoos impact a visitor’s behaviors, perceptions, and conservation efforts. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 1746. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01746

Grajal, A., Luebke, J. F., Kelly, L. A. D., Matiasek, J., Clayton, S., Karazsia, B. T., Saunders, C. D., Goldman, S. R., Mann, M. E., & Stanoss, R. (2017). The complex relationship between personal sense of connection to animals and self-reported proenvironmental behaviors by zoo visitors. Conservation Biology, 31(2), 322-330. https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.12780

Harley, H. E., Bauer, G., & Cook, P. (2022, July 31). Exploring marine mammal cognition as a conservation tool. Workshop presented at 24th Biennial Conference on the Biology of Marine Animals, Palm Beach, FL.

Harley, H. E., Fellner, W., & Stamper, M. A. (2010). Cognitive research with dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) at Disney’s The Seas: A program for enrichment, science, education, and conservation. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23, 331–343. https://doi.org/10.46867/IJCP.2010.23.03.06

Highfill, L., & Kuczaj, S. (2010). How studies of wild and captive dolphins contribute to our understanding of individual differences and personality. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23(3). https://doi.org/10.46867/ijcp.2010.23.03.09

Highfill, L., & Yeater, D. (2018). Engaging undergraduates in comparative psychology: A case study. International Journal of Comparative Psychology31. https://doi.org/10.46867/ijcp.2018.31.01.06

Hill, H. M., & Karlin, M. (2019). Reflections on an international research immersion field study as a high impact practice to produce publishable papers by underrepresented undergraduates. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 601. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00601

Hill, H. M., Walker, R., Eskelinen, H., & Trone, M. (Eds.). (2016). A Legacy of Research Inspired by Dr. Stan Kuczaj (1950 – 2016) A Special Issue – Part 1. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 29.

Hill, H. M., Weiss, M., Brasseur, I., Manibusan, A., & (2022). Killer whale innovation: Teaching animals to use their creativity upon request. Animal Cognition, 25, 1091–1108.

Hill, H., Yeater, D., Gallop, S., Guarino, S., Lacy, S., Dees, T., & Kuczaj, S. (2016).  Responses to familiar and unfamiliar humans by belugas (Delphinapterus leucas), bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), & Pacific white-sided dolphins (Lagenorhynchus obliquidens): A replication and extension. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 29. https://doi.org/10.46867/ijcp.2016.29.00.09

Hill, H., Yeater, D., & Noonan, M. (2021). Synergy between behavioral research on beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas) conducted in zoological and wild settings. Polar Research, 40, Article 5508. https://doi.org/10.33265/polar.v40.5508

Hopper, L. M. (2017). Cognitive research in zoos. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 16, 100–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.04.006

Horback, K. M., & Parsons, T. D. (2022). Judgment bias of group housed gestating sows predicted by behavioral traits, but not physical measures of welfare. PLOS ONE, 17(2), Article e0264258. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264258

Inside Edition. (2020, December 5). How Bunny the Talking Dog is teaching people to communicate with their pets. https://www.insideedition.com/how-bunny-the-talking-dog-is-teaching-people-to-communicate-with-their-pets-63284

Jaakkola, K. (2023). Are dolphins kept in impoverished environments? Animals, 13(17), Article 2707. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0013787

Karenina, K., Giljov, A., Baranov, V., Osipova, L., Kransnova, V., & Malashichev, Y. (2010). Visual laterality of calf-mother interactions in wild whales. PLOS ONE, 5(11), Article e13787. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0013787

Karenina, K., Giljov, A., Ivkovick, T., Burdin, A., & Malashichev, Y. (2013). Lateralization of spatial relationships between wild mother and infant orcas, Orcinus orca. Animal Behaviour, 86, 1225–1231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.09.025

Karenina, K. A., Giljov, A. N., Malashichev, Y. B., Baranov, V. S., & Belkovich, V. M. (2010). Visual lateralization in wild: Perceiving of novel object in beluga whale (Delphinapterus leucas). Journal of Asymmetry4, 3–12.

Karlin, M. L., Hill, H. M. M. & Alam, F. B. (2022). An immersive field study as an undergraduate research opportunity and additive high-impact practice (HIP) experience for underrepresented students. SN Social Sciences, 2(5), Article 58. https://doi.org/10.1007/s43545-022-00364-1

Krasheninnikova, A., Chow, P. K. Y., & von Bayern, A. M. P. (2020). Comparative cognition: Practical shortcomings and some potential ways forward. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74(3), 160–169. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000204

Kuczaj, S. (2010). Research with captive marine mammals is important: An introduction to the special issue. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23(3). https://doi.org/10.46867/IJCP.2010.23.03.12

Lambert, M. L., Farrar, B. G., Garcia-Pelegrin, E., Reber, S., & Miller, R. (2022). ManyBirds: A multi-site collaborative Open Science approach to avian cognition and behavior research. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 9(1), 133–152. https://doi.org/10.26451/abc.09.01.11.2022

Lanning, S., & Brown, M. (2019). Undergraduate research as a high impact practice in higher education. Education Sciences, 9(3), Article 160. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci9030160

Lilley, M. K., de Vere, A. J., & Yeater, D. B. (2020). Laterality of eye use by bottlenose (Tursiops truncatus) and rough-toothed (Steno bredanensis) dolphins while viewing predictable and unpredictable stimuli. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 33. https://doi.org/10.46867/ijcp.2020.33.03.01

Lilley, M. K., de Vere, A. J., Yeater, D. B., & Kuczaj, S. A., II. (2018). Characterizing curiosity-related behavior in bottlenose (Tursiops truncatus) and rough-toothed (Steno bredanensis) dolphins: Individual differences and implications for environmental enrichment.  International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 31. https://doi.org/10.46867/ijcp.2018.31.04.07

Manitzas Hill, H., Guarino, S., Yeater, D., White, C., Kolodziej, K., Ali, S., Lamia, S., Garcia, E., Dear, M., Halter, E., & Bradley, M. (2022). Cetacean responses to violation of expectation paradigm in a freeswim context. Animal Cognition, 26(2), 667–686. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-022-01704-7

Maple, T. L. (2016). The rise and fall of animal behavior labs: The future of comparative psychology. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 3(3), 131–143. https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.02.08.2016

ManyDogs Project; Alberghina, D., Bray, E., Buchsbaum, D., Byosiere, S.-E., Espinosa, J., Gnanadesikan, G., Guran, C.-N. A., Hare, E., Horschler, D., Huber, L., Kuhlmeier, V. A., MacLean, E., Pelgrim, M. H., Perez, B., Ravid-Schurr, D., Rothkoff, L., Sexton, C., Silver, Z., & Stevens, J. R. (2023). ManyDogs Project: A big team science approach to investigating canine behavior and cognition. Comparative Cognition and Behavior Reviews, 18, 59–77. https://doi.org/10.3819/CCBR.2023.180004

ManyPrimates; Aguenounon, G., Allritz, M., Altschul, D. M., Ballesta, S., Beaud, A., Bohn, M., Bornbusch, S. L., Brandão, A., Brooks, J., Bugnyar, T., Burkart, J. M., Bustamante, L., Call, J., Canteloup, C., Cao, C., Caspar, K. R., da Silva, D., de Sousa, A. A., DeTroy, S. E., … Zablocki-Thomas, P. (2022). The evolution of primate short-term memory. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 9(4), 428–516. https://doi.org/10.26451/abc.09.04.06.2022

ManyPrimates; Altschul, D. M., Beran, M. J., Bohn, M., Call, J., DeTroy, S., Duguid, S. J., Egelkamp, C. L., Fichtel, C., Fischer, J., Flessert, M., Hanus, D., Haun, D. B. M., Haux, L. M., Hernandez-Aguilar, R. A., Herrmann, E., Hopper, L. M., Joly, M., Kano, F., Keupp, S., … Watzek, J., (2019) Establishing an infrastructure for collaboration in primate cognition research. PLOS ONE, 14(10), Article e0223675. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223675

Matsubara, S., Deeming, D. C., & Wilkinson, A. (2017). Cold-blooded cognition: New directions in reptile cognition. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 16, 126–130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.06.006

Mehrkam, L. R. (2020). The cognitive abilities of wild animals. In V. A. Melfi, N. R. Dorey, & S. J. Ward (Eds.), Zoo animal learning and training (pp. 15–34). Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118968543.ch2

Mellor, D. J., Beausoleil, N. J., Littlewood, K. E., McLean, A. N., McGreevy, P. D., Jones, B., & Wilkins, C. (2020). The 2020 five domains model: Including human–animal interactions in assessments of animal welfare. Animals, 10(10), Article 1870. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10101870

Melzer, D., Yeater, D., Bradley, M., Manitzas Hill, H., Guerra, G., Salazar, E., Bolton, T., & Dudzinski, K. (2022). A comparative test of creative thinking in preschool children and dolphins. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 9(3), 349–362. https://doi.org/10.26451/abc.09.03.07.2022

Miller, L. J., Mellen, J. D., & Kuczaj II, S. A. (2013). The importance of behavioral research in zoological institutions: An introduction to the special issue. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 26(1). https://doi.org/10.46867/ijcp.2013.26.01.09

Nawroth, C., Langbein, M., Coulon, M., Gabor, V., Oesterwind, S., Benz-Schwarzburg, J., & von Borell, E. (2019). Farm animal cognition—Linking behavior, welfare and ethics, Frontiers in Veterinary Science, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2019.00024

Pritchard, D. J., Hurly, T. A., Tello-Ramos, M. C., & Healy, S. D. (2016). Why study cognition in the wild (and how to test it)? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 105(1), 41–55. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.195

Salzer, A. R., & Reed, D. D. (2022). The ideal free distribution accounts for free-operant domesticated dog foraging. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior118(2), 327–336. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.785

Shettleworth, S. J. (2009). Cognition, evolution, and behavior. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195319842.001.0001

Siniscalchi, M., Dimatteo, S., Pepe, A., Sasso, R., & Quaranta, A. (2012). Visual lateralization in wild striped dolphins (Stenella coeruleoalba) in response to stimuli with different degrees of familiarity. PLOS ONE, 7(1), Article e30001. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030001

Stewart, L., MacLean, E. L., Ivy, D., Woods, V., Cohen, E., Rodriguez, K., McIntyre, M., Mukherjee, S., Call, J., Kaminski, J., Miklósi, A., Wrangham, R. W., & Hare, B. (2015). Citizen science as a new tool in dog cognition research. PLOS ONE, 10(9), Article e0135176. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0135176

Sueur, C., & Pelé, M. (2023). Editorial: Recent advances in animal cognition and ethology. Animals, 13, Article 2890. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13182890

The Ranch. (2021, November 4). Teaching elephants to protect themselves from poachers. https://theranch.clickertraining.com/live-episode-48/

Thielke, L. E., & Udell, M. A. (2019). Evaluating cognitive and behavioral outcomes in conjunction with the secure base effect for dogs in shelter and foster environments. Animals, 9(11), Article 932. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9110932

Trisko, R. K., & Smuts, B. B. (2015). Dominance relationships in a group of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Behaviour, 152(5), 677–704. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003249

Vonk, J., Hoffmaster, E., Johnson-Ulrich, Z., & Oriani, S. (2015). If we build it comparative psychologists will come. Commentary: A crisis in comparative psychology: Where have all the undergraduates gone? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 1500. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01700

Willgohs, K., Williams, J., Franklin, E., & Highfill, L. (2022). The creative canine: Investigating the concept of creativity in dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) using citizen science. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 35https://doi.org/10.46867/IJCP.2023.35.5621

Wynne, C. (2016). What is special about dog cognition? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(5), 345–350. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416657540

Yeater, D. B., Guarino, S., Lacy, S., Dees, T., & Hill, H. (2017). Do belugas (Delphinapterus leucas), bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), & Pacific white-sided dolphins (Lagenorhynchus obliquidens) display lateralized eye preference when presented with familiar or novel objects? International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 30. https://doi.org/10.46867/ijcp.2017.30.00.02

Yeater, D. B., Hill, H. M., Baus, N., Farnell, H., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2014). Visual laterality in belugas (Delphinapterus leucas) and Pacific white-sided dolphins (Lagenorhynchus obliquidens) when viewing familiar and unfamiliar humans. Animal Cognition, 17, 1245–1259. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0756-x

Yeater, D., B., Melzer, D., & Highfill, L. (2024). Violation of expectation task in Canis lupus familiaris [Unpublished raw data].